Motion-driven enhancement of a lower region cue in depth perception

  • Yuki Kubota Information Somatics Laboratory, RCAST, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan; and NTT Communication Science Laboratory, Kanagawa, Japan
  • Ryota Mima Information Somatics Laboratory, RCAST, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
  • Takahiro Kawabe NTT Communication Science Laboratory, Kanagawa, Japan
  • Taiki Fukiage NTT Communication Science Laboratory, Kanagawa, Japan
  • Masahiko Inami Information Somatics Laboratory, RCAST, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
Keywords: depth illusion, depth order, lower region cue, motion-defined boundary

Abstract

The authors report a demonstration in which a motion-defined boundary enhances the effects of a lower region cue in depth perception. Although the lower region cue has been proposed as a potential depth cue, its effect is weak in the static image. Their demonstration reveals that the lower region is almost unambiguously perceived as being in front when defined by horizontal motion mimicking motion parallax. The authors further investigated phenomenological aspects of the lower region cue by combining it with other depth cues.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References


Bonaiuto, J. J., de Berker, A., & Bestmann, S. (2016). Response repetition biases in human perceptual decisions are explained by activity decay in competitive attractor models. Elife, 5, e20047. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20047.025


Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (1981). Velocity gradients and relative depth perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 29(2), 145–155. doi: 10.3758/BF03207278


Grossberg, S. (1997). Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional figure–ground perception of two- dimensional pictures. Psychological Review, 104(3), 618–658. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.3.618


Hulleman, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). A new cue to figure–ground coding: Top–bottom polarity. Vision Research, 44(24), 2779–2791. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.06.012


Kaneko, H., & Uchikawa, K. (1993). Apparent relative size and depth of moving objects. Perception, 22(5), 537–547. doi: 10.1068/p220537


Kaplan, G. A. (1969). Kinetic disruption of optical texture: The perception of depth at an edge. Perception & Psychophysics, 6(4), 193–198. doi: 10.3758/BF03207015


Kitazaki, M., & Shimojo, S. (1998). Surface discontinuity is critical in a moving observer’s perception of objects depth order and relative motion from retinal image motion. Perception, 27(10), 1153–1176. doi: 10.1068/p271153


Necker, L. A. (1832). Lxi. observations on some remarkable optical phenomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid. The London, Edinburgh, and Dublin Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 1(5), 329–337. doi: 10.1080/14786443208647909


Sun, J., & Perona, P. (1998). Where is the sun? Nature Neuroscience, 1(3), 183–184. doi: 10.1038/630


Vecera, S. P., & Palmer, S. E. (2006). Grounding the figure: Surface attachment inuences figure-ground organization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 563–569. doi: 10.3758/BF03193963


Vecera, S. P., P., Vogel, E. K., & Woodman, G. F. (2002). Lower region: A new cue for figure-ground assignment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(2), 194–205. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.2.194
Published
2022-04-14
How to Cite
Kubota Y., Mima R., Kawabe T., Fukiage T., & Inami M. (2022). Motion-driven enhancement of a lower region cue in depth perception. Journal of Illusion, 3. https://doi.org/10.47691/joi.v3.8028
Section
Phenomenal reports