A review of the footsteps illusion

Keywords: footsteps illusion, motion perception, contrast, geometrical illusion, extinction effect, position capture, motion capture, reverse phi

Abstract

Studies on the footsteps illusion proposed by Anstis (2001) and its variants are reviewed in this article. The footsteps illusion has been explained as a difference in perceived speed depending on edge contrast (Thompson, 1982). In addition to this explanation, it is suggested that the footsteps illusion and its variants can also be attributed to the geometrical illusion presented by Gregory and Heard (1983), to the extinction effect similar to hidden images by Wade (1990), and to subsequent position or motion captures. Related illusions, for example, the kickback illusion (Howe, Thompson, Anstis, Sagreiya, & Livingstone, 2006), the kick-forward illusion, the driving-on-a-bumpy-road illusion, or the footsteps illusion based upon reverse phi motion, are discussed in this article.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References


Anstis, S. M. (1970). Phi movement as a subtraction process. Vision Research, 10, 1411–1430. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(70)90092-1


Anstis, S. M. (2001). Footsteps and inchworms: Illusions show that contrast modulates motion salience. Perception, 30, 785–794. doi: 10.1068/p3211


Anstis, S. M. (2003). Moving objects appear to slow down at low contrasts. Neural Networks, 16, 933–938. doi: 10.1068/p140167


Anstis, S. M. (2004). Factors affecting footsteps: Contrast can change the apparent speed, amplitude and direction of motion. Vision Research, 44, 2171–2178. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.03.015


Anstis, S. M., & Rogers, B. J. (1975). Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast. Vision Research, 15, 957–961. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90236-9


Anstis, S. M., & Rogers, B. J. (1986). Illusory continuous motion from oscillating positive–negative patterns: Implications for motion perception. Perception, 15, 627–640. doi: 10.1068/p150627


Anstis, S., Verstraten, F. A., & Mather, G. (1998). The motion aftereffect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(3), 111–117. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01142-5


Goda, N., & Ejima, Y. (1997). Moving stimuli define the shape of stationary chromatic patterns. Perception, 26, 1413–1422. doi: 10.1068/p261413


Goldberg, D. M., & Pomerantz, J. R. (1982). Models of illusory pausing and sticking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(4), 547–561. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.8.4.547


Gregory, R. L., & Heard, P. F. (1983). Visual dissociations of movement, position, and stereo depth. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 35, 217–237. doi: 10.1080/14640748308402127


Howe, P. D. L., Thompson, P. G., Anstis, S., Sagreiya, H., & Livingstone, M. S. (2006). Explaining the footsteps, belly dancer, Wenceslas and kickback illusions. Journal of Vision, 6, 1396–1405. doi: 10.1167/6.12.5


Kitaoka, A. (2006). Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception, 35, 799–806. doi: 10.1068/p5319b


Kitaoka, A. (2014). Color-dependent motion illusions in stationary images and their phenomenal dimorphism. Perception, 43(9), 914–925. doi: 10.1068/p7706


Kitaoka, A. (2017). The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension. In A. G. Shapiro & D. Todorović (Eds.), The Oxford compendium of visual illusions (pp. 500–511). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794607.003.0068


Kitaoka, A., & Anstis, S. (2015). Second-order footsteps illusions. i-Perception, 6(6), 1–4. doi: 10.1177/2041669515622085


Kitaoka, A., & Ashida, H. (2007). A variant of the anomalous motion illusion based upon contrast and visual latency. Perception, 36, 1019–1035. doi: 10.1068/p5362


Murakami, I., & Shimojo, S. (1993). Motion capture changes to induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentricities, and larger inducer sizes. Vision Research, 33, 2091–2107. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90008-K


Pinna, B., & Brelstaff, G. J. (2000). A new visual illusion of relative motion. Vision Research, 40, 2091–2096. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00072-9


Ramachandran, V. S. (1987). Interaction between colour and motion in human vision. Nature, 328, 645–647. doi: 10.1038/328645a0


Rogers, B., Anstis, S., Ashida, H., & Kitaoka, A. (2019). Reversed phi and the ‘phenomenal phenomena’ revisited. i-Perception, 10(4), 1–22. doi: 10.1177/2041669519856906


Spillmann, L., Saito, K., & Komatsu, H. (2016). Hajime Ōuchi – A mystery resolved. Perception, 45, 371–374. doi: 10.1177/0301006616637433


Stone, L. S., & Thompson, P. (1992). Human speed perception is contrast dependent. Vision Research, 32, 1535–1549. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90209-2


Sunaga, S., Sato, M., Arikado, N., & Jomoto, H. (2008). A static geometrical illusion contributes largely to the footsteps illusion. Perception, 37, 902–914. doi: 10.1068/p5689


Thompson, P. (1982). Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast. Vision Research, 22, 377–380. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90153-5


Thompson, P., & Anstis, S. (2005). Retracing our footsteps: A revised theory of the footsteps illusion [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 5(8), 929. doi: 10.1167/5.8.929


Wade, N. (1990). Visual allusions: Pictures of perception. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.


Wade, N. J. (2017). Hidden images. In A. G. Shapiro & D. Todorović (Eds.), The Oxford compendium of visual illusions (pp. 774–780). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794607.003.0113


Zenger-Landolt, B., & Koch, C. (2001). Flanker effects in peripheral contrast discrimination – Psychophysics and modeling. Vision Research, 41, 3663–3675. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00175-4
Published
2021-03-22
How to Cite
Kitaoka A., & Anstis S. (2021). A review of the footsteps illusion. Journal of Illusion, 2. https://doi.org/10.47691/joi.v2.5612
Section
Review reports